Binaural beats
From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Please help improve this article or section by expanding it. Further information might be found on the talk page. (January 2009) |
This article needs additional citations for verification. Please help improve this article by adding reliable references (ideally, using inline citations). Unsourced material may be challenged and removed. (December 2007) |
Binaural beats or binaural tones are auditory processing artifacts, or apparent sounds, the perception of which arises in the brain independent of physical stimuli. This effect was discovered in 1839 by Heinrich Wilhelm Dove.
The brain produces a phenomenon resulting in low-frequency pulsations in the loudness of a perceived sound when two tones at slightly different frequencies are presented separately, one to each of a subject's ears, using stereo headphones. A beating tone will be perceived, as if the two tones mixed naturally, out of the brain. The frequency of the tones must be below about 1,000 to 1,500 hertz for the beating to be heard. The difference between the two frequencies must be small (below about 30 Hz) for the effect to occur; otherwise, the two tones will be heard separately and no beat will be perceived.
Binaural beats are of interest to neurophysiologists investigating the sense of hearing. Second (and more controversially), binaural beats reportedly influence the brain in more subtle ways through the entrainment of brainwaves[1][2] and can be used to produce relaxation and other health benefits such as pain relief.[3]
Contents |
[edit] History
Heinrich Wilhelm Dove discovered binaural beats in 1839. While research about them continued after that, the subject remained somewhat of a scientific curiosity until 134 years later, with the publishing of Gerald Oster's article "Auditory Beats in the Brain" (Scientific American, 1973). Oster's article identified and assembled the scattered islands of relevant research since Dove, offering tremendous fresh insight (and new laboratory findings) to research on binaural beats.
In particular, Oster saw binaural beats as a powerful tool for cognitive and neurological research, addressing questions such as how animals locate sounds in their three-dimensional environment, and also the remarkable ability of animals to pick out and focus on specific sounds in a sea of noise (what is known as the "cocktail party effect").
Oster also considered binaural beats to be a potentially useful medical diagnostic tool, not merely for finding and assessing auditory impairments, but also for more general neurological conditions. (Binaural beats involve different neurological pathways than ordinary auditory processing.) For example, Oster found that a number of his subjects that could not perceive binaural beats suffered from Parkinson's disease. In one particular case, Oster was able to follow the subject through a week-long treatment of Parkinson's disease; at the outset the patient could not perceive binaural beats; but by the end of the week of treatment, the patient was able to hear them.
In corroborating an earlier study, Oster also reported gender differences in the perception of beats. Specifically, women seemed to experience two separate peaks in their ability to perceive binaural beats- peaks possibly correlating with specific points in the menstrual cycle (onset of menstruation and approx. 15 after). This data led Oster to wonder if binaural beats could be used as a tool for measuring relative levels of estrogen.[1]
The effects of binaural beats on consciousness were first examined by physicists Thomas Campbell and Dennis Mennerich, who under the direction of Robert Monroe sought to reproduce a subjective impression of 4hz oscillation that they associated with out of body experience.[4] On the strength of their findings, Monroe spawned the binaural self-development industry by forming The Monroe Institute, now a charitable binaural research and education organization.
[edit] Physiology
The sensation of binaural beats is believed to originate in the superior olivary nucleus, a part of the brain stem. They appear to be related to the brain's ability to locate the sources of sounds in three dimensions and to track moving sounds, which also involves inferior colliculus (IC) neurons.[5] Regarding entrainment, the study of rhythmicity provides insights into the understanding of temporal information processing in the human brain. Auditory rhythms rapidly entrain motor responses into stable steady synchronization states below and above conscious perception thresholds. Activated regions include primary sensorimotor and cingulate areas, bilateral opercular premotor areas, bilateral SII, ventral prefrontal cortex, and, subcortically, anterior insula, putamen, and thalamus. Within the cerebellum, vermal regions and anterior hemispheres ipsilateral to the movement became significantly activated. Tracking temporal modulations additionally activated predominantly right prefrontal, anterior cingulate, and intraparietal regions as well as posterior cerebellar hemispheres.[6] A study of aphasic subjects who had a severe stroke versus normal subjects showed that the aphasic subject could not hear the binaural beats whereas the normal subjects could.[7]
[edit] Hypothetical effects on brain function
This article requires authentication or verification by an expert. Please assist in recruiting an expert or improve this article yourself. See the talk page for details. (February 2009) |
[edit] Overview
Binaural beats may influence functions of the brain besides those related to hearing. This phenomenon is called frequency following response. The concept is that if one receives a stimulus with a frequency in the range of brain waves, the predominant brain wave frequency is said to be likely to move towards the frequency of the stimulus (a process called entrainment).[8] In addition, binaural beats have been credibly documented to relate to both spatial perception & stereo auditory recognition, and, according to the frequency following response, activation of various sites in the brain.[9][10][11][12][13]
The stimulus does not have to be aural; it can also be visual[14] or a combination of aural and visual.[15] (One such example would be Dreamachine.) However, using alpha frequencies with such stimuli can trigger photosensitive epilepsy.
Perceived human hearing is limited to the range of frequencies from 20 Hz to 20,000 Hz, though Infrasound - sound below 20Hz - still have scientifically observable effects on humans, however, it is not readily audible, especially at low volume levels. While the frequencies of human brain waves are below about 40 Hz. To account for this lack of perception, binaural beat frequencies are used. Beat frequencies of 40 Hz have been produced in the brain with binaural sound and measured experimentally.[16]
When the perceived beat frequency corresponds to the delta, theta, alpha, beta, or gamma range of brainwave frequencies, the brainwaves entrain to or move towards the beat frequency.[17] For example, if a 315 Hz sine wave is played into the right ear and a 325 Hz one into the left ear, the brain is entrained towards the beat frequency (10 Hz, in the alpha range. Since alpha range is associated with relaxation, this has a relaxing effect or if in the beta range, more alertness. An experiment with binaural sound stimulation using beat frequencies in the Beta range on some participants and Delta/Theta range in other participants, found better vigilance performance and mood in those on the awake alert state of Beta range stimulation.[18][19]
Binaural beat stimulation has been used fairly extensively to induce a variety of states of consciousness, and there has been some work done in regards to the effects of these stimuli on relaxation, focus, attention, and states of consciousness.[20] Studies have shown that with repeated training to distinguish close frequency sounds that a plastic reorganization of the brain occurs for the trained frequencies[21] and is capable of asymmetric hemispheric balancing.[22]
[edit] Brain waves
Frequency range | Name | Usually associated with: |
---|---|---|
> 40 Hz | Gamma waves | Higher mental activity, including perception, problem solving, fear, and consciousness |
13–40 Hz | Beta waves | Active, busy or anxious thinking and active concentration, arousal, cognition |
7–13 Hz | Alpha waves | Relaxation (while awake), pre-sleep and pre-wake drowsiness |
4–7 Hz | Theta waves | Dreams, deep meditation, REM sleep |
< 4 Hz | Delta waves | Deep dreamless sleep, loss of body awareness |
(The precise boundaries between ranges vary among definitions, and there is no universally accepted standard.)
The dominant frequency determines your current state. For example, if in someone's brain alpha waves are dominating, they are in the alpha state (this happens when one is relaxed but awake). However, also other frequencies will be present, albeit with smaller amplitudes.
The brain entraining is more effective if the entraining frequency is close to the user's starting dominant frequency. Therefore, it is suggested to start with a frequency near to one's current dominant frequency (likely to be about 20 Hz or less for a waking person), and then slowly decreasing it towards the desired frequency.
Some people find pure sine waves unpleasant, so a pink noise or another background (e.g. natural sounds such as river noises) can also be mixed with them. In addition to that, as long as the beat is audible, increasing the volume should not necessarily improve the effectiveness, therefore using a low volume is usually suggested. One theory is to reduce the volume so low that the beating should not even be clearly audible, but this does not seem to be the case (see the next paragraph).
[edit] Other uses
In addition to lowering the brain frequency to relax the listener (or to raise it to help focusing), there are other controversial, alleged uses for binaural beats. For example, that by using specific frequencies an individual can stimulate certain glands to produce desired hormones. Beta-endorphin has been modulated in studies using alpha-theta brain wave training,[23] and dopamine with binaural beats.[24] Among other alleged uses, there are reducing learning time and sleeping needs (theta waves are thought to improve learning, since children, who have stronger theta waves, and remain in this state for a longer period of time than adults, usually learn faster than adults;[citation needed] and some people find that half an hour in the theta state can reduce sleeping needs up to four hours;[citation needed] however, this is supposed to happen with any way to get into theta state, e.g. meditation;[citation needed]) some use them for lucid dreaming and even for attempting out-of-body experiences, astral projection, telepathy and psychokinesis. However, the role of alpha-wave activity in lucid dreaming is subject to ongoing research.)[25][26][27]
Alpha-theta brainwave training has also been used successfully for the treatment of addictions,[23][28][29] for the recovery of repressed memories, but as with other techniques this can lead to false memories.[30]
A trial of Delta binaural beat technology over 60 days has shown positive effect on self-reported psychologic measures, especially anxiety. There was significant decrease in trait anxiety, an increase in quality of life, and a decrease in insulin-like growth factor-1 and dopamine[24] and has been successfully shown to decrease mild anxiety[31] and lessen hospital acute pre-operative anxiety.[32]
Another claimed effect for sound induced brain synchronization is enhanced learning ability. It was proposed in the 1970s that induced alpha brain waves enabled students to assimilate more information with greater long term retention.[33] In more recent times has come more understanding of the role of theta brain waves in behavioural learning[34] The presence of theta patterns in the brain has been associated with increased receptivity for learning and decreased filtering by the left hemisphere.[33][35][36] Based on the association between theta activity (4–7 Hz) and working memory performance, biofeedback training suggests that normal healthy individuals can learn to increase a specific component of their EEG activity, and that such enhanced activity may facilitate a working memory task and to a lesser extent focused attention.[37]
[edit] Example
For these examples to be effective, it is required that the listener use headphones. WARNING: play the files before putting on the headphones in order to set a safe volume, as the onset of the sound is quite abrupt and, especially in the second file, quite loud at first.
|
|||||
Problems listening to this file? See media help. |
|
|||||
Problems listening to this file? See media help. |
Direct downloads of the above files: Media:Binbeat sample.ogg Media:Binbeats2.ogg
[edit] See also
- Brainwave synchronization
- Electroencephalography
- Event-related potential
- Evoked potential
- Gnaural
- Hemi-Sync
- Human enhancement
- Induced activity
- Intelligence amplification
- Lucid dream
- Mind machine
- Neural oscillations
- Neurofeedback
- Ongoing brain activity
- Schumann resonances
[edit] References
- ^ a b Oster G (1973). "Auditory beats in the brain". Sci. Am. 229 (4): 94–102. PMID 4727697.
- ^ Hutchison, Michael M. (1986). Megabrain: new tools and techniques for brain growth and mind expansion. New York: W. Morrow. ISBN 0-688-04880-3.
- ^ Hemispheric-synchronisation during anaesthesia: a double-blind randomised trial using audiotapes for intra-operative nociception control, Jan 2000, Kliempt, Ruta, Ogston, Landeck & Martay
- ^ "My Big TOE" book 1, Thomas Campbell, p79 ISBN 978-0972509404
- ^ Spitzer MW, Semple MN (1998). "Transformation of binaural response properties in the ascending auditory pathway: influence of time-varying interaural phase disparity". J. Neurophysiol. 80 (6): 3062–76. PMID 9862906.
- ^ Thaut MH (2003). "Neural basis of rhythmic timing networks in the human brain". Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 999: 364–73. doi: . PMID 14681157.
- ^ Barr DF, Mullin TA, Herbert PS. (1977). "Application of binaural beat phenomenon with aphasic patients". Arch Otolaryngol. 103 (4): 192–194. PMID 849195.
- ^ Gerken GM, Moushegian G, Stillman RD, Rupert AL (1975). "Human frequency-following responses to monaural and binaural stimuli". Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology 38 (4): 379–86. doi: . PMID 46818.
- ^ Dobie RA, Norton SJ (1980). "Binaural interaction in human auditory evoked potentials". Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology 49 (3-4): 303–13. doi: . PMID 6158406.
- ^ Moushegian G, Rupert AL, Stillman RD (1978). "Evaluation of frequency-following potentials in man: masking and clinical studies". Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology 45 (6): 711–18. doi: . PMID 84739.
- ^ Smith JC, Marsh JT, Greenberg S, Brown WS (1978). "Human auditory frequency-following responses to a missing fundamental". Science 201 (4356): 639–41. doi: . PMID 675250.
- ^ Smith JC, Marsh JT, Brown WS (1975). "Far-field recorded frequency-following responses: evidence for the locus of brainstem sources". Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology 39 (5): 465–72. doi: . PMID 52439.
- ^ Yamada O, Yamane H, Kodera K (1977). "Simultaneous recordings of the brain stem response and the frequency-following response to low-frequency tone". Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology 43 (3): 362–70. doi: . PMID 70337.
- ^ Cvetkovic D, Simpson D, Cosic I (2006). "Influence of sinusoidally modulated visual stimuli at extremely low frequency range on the human EEG activity". Conference proceedings : ... Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society. IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society. Conference 1: 1311–4. doi: . PMID 17945633.
- ^ "[Abstract The Induced Rhythmic Oscillations of Neural Activity in the Human Brain"]. http://www.actapress.com/Abstract.aspx?paperId=16352. Retrieved on 2007-11-14.
- ^ Schwarz DW, Taylor P (2005). "Human auditory steady state responses to binaural and monaural beats". Clinical neurophysiology : official journal of the International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology 116 (3): 658–68. doi: . PMID 15721080.
- ^ Rogers LJ, Walter DO (1981). "Methods for finding single generators, with application to auditory driving of the human EEG by complex stimuli". J. Neurosci. Methods 4 (3): 257–65. doi: . PMID 7300432.
- ^ Lane JD, Kasian SJ, Owens JE, Marsh GR (1998). "Binaural auditory beats affect vigilance performance and mood". Physiol. Behav. 63 (2): 249–52. doi: . PMID 9423966.
- ^ Beatty J, Greenberg A, Deibler WP, O'Hanlon JF (1974). "Operant control of occipital theta rhythm affects performance in a radar monitoring task". Science 183 (127): 871–3. doi: . PMID 4810845.
- ^ Hutchison, Michael M. (1986). Megabrain: new tools and techniques for brain growth and mind expansion. New York: W. Morrow. ISBN 0-688-04880-3.
- ^ Menning H, Roberts LE, Pantev C (2000). "Plastic changes in the auditory cortex induced by intensive frequency discrimination training". Neuroreport 11 (4): 817–22. doi: . PMID 10757526.
- ^ Gottselig JM, Brandeis D, Hofer-Tinguely G, Borbély AA, Achermann P (2004). "Human central auditory plasticity associated with tone sequence learning". Learn. Mem. 11 (2): 162–71. doi: . PMID 15054131.
- ^ a b Peniston EG, Kulkosky PJ (1989). "Alpha-theta brainwave training and beta-endorphin levels in alcoholics". Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 13 (2): 271–9. doi: . PMID 2524976.
- ^ a b Wahbeh H, Calabrese C, Zwickey H (2007). "Binaural beat technology in humans: a pilot study to assess psychologic and physiologic effects". Journal of alternative and complementary medicine (New York, N.Y.) 13 (1): 25–32. doi: . PMID 17309374.
- ^ Ogilvie RD, Hunt HT, Tyson PD, Lucescu ML, Jeakins DB (1982). "Lucid dreaming and alpha activity: a preliminary report". Perceptual and motor skills 55 (3 Pt 1): 795–808. PMID 7162915.
- ^ Korabel'nikova EA, Golubev VL (2001). "[Dreams and interhemispheric asymmetry]" (in Russian). Zhurnal nevrologii i psikhiatrii imeni S.S. Korsakova / Ministerstvo zdravookhraneniia i meditsinskoĭ promyshlennosti Rossiĭskoĭ Federatsii, Vserossiĭskoe obshchestvo nevrologov Vserossiĭskoe obshchestvo psikhiatrov 101 (12): 51–4. PMID 11811128.
- ^ Spoormaker VI, van den Bout J (2006). "Lucid dreaming treatment for nightmares: a pilot study". Psychotherapy and psychosomatics 75 (6): 389–94. doi: . PMID 17053341.
- ^ Saxby E, Peniston EG (1995). "Alpha-theta brainwave neurofeedback training: an effective treatment for male and female alcoholics with depressive symptoms". Journal of clinical psychology 51 (5): 685–93. doi: . PMID 8801245.
- ^ Watson CG, Herder J, Passini FT (1978). "Alpha biofeedback therapy in alcoholics: an 18-month follow-up". Journal of clinical psychology 34 (3): 765–9. doi: . PMID 690224.
- ^ Loftus EF, Davis D (2006). "Recovered memories". Annual review of clinical psychology 2: 469–98. doi: . PMID 17716079.
- ^ Le Scouarnec RP, Poirier RM, Owens JE, Gauthier J, Taylor AG, Foresman PA. (2001). "Use of binaural beat tapes for treatment of anxiety: a pilot study of tape preference and outcomes". Altern Ther Health Med. (Clinique Psych in Montreal, Quebec.) 7 (1): 58–63. PMID 11191043.
- ^ Padmanabhan R, Hildreth AJ, Laws D (2005). "A prospective, randomised, controlled study examining binaural beat audio and pre-operative anxiety in patients undergoing general anaesthesia for day case surgery". Anaesthesia 60 (9): 874–7. doi: . PMID 16115248.
- ^ a b Harris, Bill (2002). Thresholds of the Mind. Centerpointe Press. Appendix 1, pp151–178. ISBN 0-9721780-0-7.
- ^ Berry SD, Seager MA (2001). "Hippocampal theta oscillations and classical conditioning". Neurobiol Learn Mem 76 (3): 298–313. doi: . PMID 11726239.
- ^ Seager MA, Johnson LD, Chabot ES, Asaka Y, Berry SD (2002). "Oscillatory brain states and learning: Impact of hippocampal theta-contingent training". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99 (3): 1616–20. doi: . PMID 11818559.
- ^ Griffin AL, Asaka Y, Darling RD, Berry SD (2004). "Theta-contingent trial presentation accelerates learning rate and enhances hippocampal plasticity during trace eyeblink conditioning". Behav. Neurosci. 118 (2): 403–11. doi: . PMID 15113267.
- ^ Vernon D, Egner T, Cooper N, et al (2003). "The effect of training distinct neurofeedback protocols on aspects of cognitive performance". International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology 47 (1): 75–85. PMID 12543448.
[edit] External links
This article's external links may not follow Wikipedia's content policies or guidelines. Please improve this article by removing excessive or inappropriate external links. |